Advertisement
Guest User

felconrtrol

a guest
Dec 10th, 2019
154
0
Never
Not a member of Pastebin yet? Sign Up, it unlocks many cool features!
text 16.10 KB | None | 0 0
  1. Emerging research on self-control reveals that one important factor
  2. in determining whether or not people sustain control is their experiences of effort while exerting control. In three studies, we examined
  3. how these experiences of effort might also have broader effects in terms
  4. of altering people's fundamental beliefs about the nature of self-control.
  5. Results of all three studies indicated that self-control experiences
  6. characterized by high versus low perceived effort were associated with
  7. stronger endorsement of lay theories that self-control is limited and can
  8. be exhausted through continuous use.
  9. Two studies experimentally manipulated whether participants reflected on a high-effort or low-effort experience of self-control (Study 1)
  10. or engaged in a high- or low-effort self-control task (Study 2); in both
  11. studies, participants endorsed lay theories of self-control as more limited in the high-effort condition. Furthermore, Study 2 indicated that
  12. the effects of condition on lay theories were uniquely mediated by
  13. perceived effort. Study 3 assessed the degree of effort and fatigue participants experienced during a two-week period of performing an assigned self-control task and examined its association with changes in
  14. lay theories of self-control following this period. Results similarly
  15. showed that participants who experienced more effort and fatigue also
  16. endorsed lay theories of self-control as more limited, regardless of what
  17. their initial lay theory had been. Thus, across three different paradigms,
  18. two that prioritized experimental control (Studies 1–2) and one that
  19. prioritized ecological validity (Study 3), we found converging evidence
  20. of the relation between perceived effort and lay theories of self-control.
  21. In addition, Study 3 provided further evidence for the downstream effects of increased experiences of effort during self-control on subsequent self-control performance. Consistent with prior research on lay
  22. theories of self-control (Bernecker & Job, 2015; Job et al., 2010; Job,
  23. Walton, et al., 2015; Miller et al., 2012), the shifts toward lay theories
  24. of self-control as limited further mediated reduced self-control performance following such experiences. Moreover, also consistent with
  25. previous research, the mediational pathway was only observed for the
  26. final self-control-related task in the experimental session when cumulative demands for control were relatively high.
  27. Interestingly, the effects of perceived effort on lay theories of selfcontrol were largely specific to the type of self-control experience that
  28. was salient. In Studies 1 and 3, in which self-control was defined for and
  29. experienced by participants as being primarily about resisting temptations, effort was related most strongly to lay theories of resisting
  30. temptation, not mental exertion. In contrast, in Study 2, in which selfcontrol was defined for participants as being primarily about mental
  31. exertion, perceived effort was related to lay theories of mental exertion,
  32. but not to lay theories of resisting temptation. These contrasting patterns of results suggest that individuals may be relying on effort signals
  33. to draw more narrow and specific inferences about their capacity in
  34. particular self-control situations, not about their self-regulatory capacity more broadly. Although more systematic research will be necessary
  35. to confirm the specificity of such inferences, our findings indicate that
  36. there is potential value in examining different lay theories about self
  37.  
  38.  
  39.  
  40.  
  41. These findings provide new insights into how the experience of selfcontrol as effortful and exhausting may affect not only proximal, but
  42. also distal, self-control outcomes. Although recent motivational accounts of self-control (e.g., Inzlicht & Schmeichel, 2012; Kurzban et al.,
  43. 2013; Molden et al., 2016, 2018) vary in a number of details, there is
  44. general recognition that whereas signals of exhaustion and effort may
  45. not reflect a literal state of resource depletion, they may still function as
  46. key signals about how much to continue to invest in goal pursuit. The
  47. current studies extend this idea further by suggesting that people may
  48. make important inferences about the nature of self-control based on
  49. these feelings of effort and exhaustion. These inferences can then influence people's lay theories about self-control and affect self-control
  50. performance in unrelated situations. The present results thus provide
  51. some initial evidence that high-effort cues can impact self-control well
  52. beyond the immediate situation. In particular, they suggest that the
  53. experience of high effort and exhaustion during self-control can itself
  54. lead to reduced self-control performance in new situations where regulatory demands are high.
  55. These results are particularly interesting in light of the conclusions
  56. drawn by Vohs, Baumeister, and Schmeichel (2012) concerning the
  57. effects of “severely depleting” experiences and the evidence this provides for the limited capacity of self-control. Vohs et al. presented a
  58. study in which they first temporarily activated either more limited or
  59. non-limited lay theories of self-control by having people rate their
  60. agreement with statements biased toward one of the two theories (see
  61. Job et al., 2010). Temporarily increasing endorsement of a non-limited
  62. versus a limited theory of self-control in this manner bolstered subsequent performance on self-control tasks when participants were presented with two consecutive self-control tasks, which was presumably
  63. experienced as moderately taxing; however, this manipulation did not
  64. affect performance when participants were presented with four consecutive self-control tasks, which was presumably experienced as extremely taxing. Vohs et al. concluded that these findings confirm that,
  65. although perceptions and interpretations of effort during self-control
  66. might affect such control in some cases, there is still a limited capacity
  67. that will be depleted if people have to sustain self-control for a long
  68. enough period of time or at high enough levels. In the context of studies
  69. reviewed earlier (Bernecker & Job, 2015; Job et al., 2010; Job, Walton,
  70. et al., 2015; Miller et al., 2012), these findings could be interpreted as
  71. suggesting that under conditions of extremely high demand, lay theories are no longer influential.
  72. The present findings, however, question the necessity of this conclusion. If the lengthier and more taxing condition of the Vohs et al.
  73. (2012) experiment was indeed experienced as more effortful and fatiguing, then these experiences could readily have shifted people's lay
  74. theories back toward a more limited view of self-control, counteracting
  75. any influence of the initial manipulation. That is, we would argue (see
  76. also Molden et al., 2016) that whatever temporary accessibility for nonlimited theories of self-control was created at the beginning of the experiment could have been overridden by the more recent and prolonged
  77. experience of effort and fatigue involved with completing four consecutive self-control-relevant tasks (see Higgins, 1996). Indeed, it is
  78. even possible that the violation of expectations experienced by participants who were encouraged to endorse non-limited theories of selfcontrol but then experienced high levels of effort and fatigue from the
  79. prolonged series of tasks they performed made the implications of these
  80. later experiences more salient. This would explain the particularly low
  81. performance observed in this condition of the experiment. Thus,
  82. overall, complementing previous findings on the role of people's perceptions and experiences in explaining results previously interpreted as
  83. a depleted capacity for self-control (e.g., Clarkson et al., 2010; Laran &
  84. Janiszewski, 2011; Moller et al., 2006; Muraven et al., 2008), the
  85.  
  86.  
  87.  
  88. results presented here suggest that even apparent instances of “severe
  89. depletion” can be understood in terms of people's perceptions and experiences of effort rather than as the exhaustion of some self-control
  90. resource.
  91. A broader question concerning the influences of effort on sustained
  92. self-control that is important for future research is whether experiences
  93. of effort during self-control are directly equivalent to experiences of
  94. fatigue. Although, colloquially, the two terms are often used interchangeably and we did not have adequate measures to distinguish these
  95. constructs in the present studies, they could be theoretically distinct in
  96. critical ways. For instance, prior work has shown that effort can—but
  97. does not necessarily—produce fatigue (e.g., Brehm & Self, 1989;
  98. Csikszentmihalyi, 1997). Indeed, Molden et al. (2016, 2018) argue that
  99. effort (i.e., sustained attention) itself may simply be one input to determining fatigue. Specifically, in their motivated effort-allocation
  100. (MEA) model of self-regulation, the relationship between effort and
  101. fatigue further depends on the perceived progress toward some desired
  102. outcome produced by the effort exerted. It is only when people's efforts
  103. are not perceived to be producing sufficient progress—i.e., when this
  104. effort does not seem to be entirely worthwhile—that fatigue is proposed
  105. to arise. Indeed, there might be cases when experiences of effort actually lead to less limited theories if perceived effort is positively correlated with perceptions of worth; under these conditions, it is possible
  106. that individuals might actually infer that effort is energizing and that
  107. their capacity for regulation is expanding or limitless (see Mrazek et al.,
  108. 2018). Thus, it will be important in future research to develop effective
  109. means of separately assessing effort versus fatigue. This could involve
  110. the development of both more sensitive self-report measures as well as
  111. the use of physiological indicators of self-regulatory effort and fatigue
  112. (e.g., Hui et al., 2009; Segerstrom & Nes, 2007).
  113. In addition, although our account of how the experience of effort
  114. influences lay theories is based on the inferences people make, the
  115. studies did not directly test the mechanism by which perceptions of
  116. effort affects lay theories. Experiences of effort might also affect what
  117. factors are most salient when reflecting on lay beliefs, may change the
  118. perceived value of effort, or may more generally affect individuals'
  119. confidence about their self-regulatory capabilities, leading them to
  120. make more conservative estimates of what they (and perhaps even
  121. others) are capable of doing. In future work it will be interesting to
  122. directly examine potential mechanisms, of which we recognize that the
  123. inference account we have proposed is only one possibility.
  124.  
  125.  
  126. Implications for improving self-control
  127. Another way to consider the present findings that experiencing selfcontrol as effortful may produce more limited lay theories of control
  128. and impair subsequent performance is the reverse: that experiencing
  129. self-control as low effort may support more non-limited theories of
  130. control and bolster performance in the face of high regulatory demands.
  131. The studies reported here do not speak directly to whether experiences
  132. of high versus low effort and exhaustion are relatively more influential
  133. in shifting lay theories, which is an interesting direction for future research to address. Nevertheless, these findings confirm the potential of
  134. methods to bolster self-control by directly altering how people experience and interpret the effort involved.
  135. Indeed, the present findings could help further explain why proactive strategies of self-control, in which people alter their environments
  136. and behaviors so as to minimize their potential of actually having to
  137. engage in effortful control, are often so effective (see Ent, Baumeister, &
  138. Tice, 2015; Fujita, 2011; Hofmann, Baumeister, Förster, & Vohs, 2012;
  139. Trope & Fishbach, 2000; Wertenbroch, 1998). If effort and fatigue are
  140. not frequently experienced, then people's broader lay theories of selfcontrol may remain more non-limited, further bolstering whatever efforts they may need to make in the future. Our findings could also help
  141. explain why strategies of engaging in consistent self-control “practice,”
  142. in contrast, may not always be effective and could sometimes even
  143.  
  144.  
  145. backfire. Although this type of intervention has shown some promise
  146. for improving self-control (e.g., Job, Friese, & Bernecker, 2015;
  147. Muraven, 2010a; Muraven, 2010b), the cumulative evidence for its
  148. effectiveness is less certain (Inzlicht & Berkman, 2015; Miles et al.,
  149. 2016). This mixed evidence could arise because the experiences that
  150. people have during self-control practice may determine how well such
  151. an intervention succeeds. Difficult self-control tasks that have a high
  152. likelihood of being experienced as effortful and exhausting may actually undermine self-control performance in the future. For instance, one
  153. study found that participants who practiced controlling their moods
  154. over a two-week period actually showed a subsequent decrease (from
  155. baseline) in self-control performance (Muraven, Baumeister, & Tice,
  156. 1999). In contrast, practice tasks such as keeping good posture (Sultan,
  157. Joireman, & Sprott, 2012) and using one's non-dominant hand for
  158. certain tasks (Denson, Capper, Oaten, Friese, & Schofield, 2011), instead improved self-control. These latter tasks may, on average, have
  159. been experienced as less effortful and exhausting compared to a practice task like controlling one's moods. Investigating the influence of
  160. different types of practice, and the role of the effort and fatigue they
  161. evoke, is thus an interesting direction for future work.
  162. Beyond the actual experience of effort itself, another important
  163. point made by the present research is that it would benefit any proposed method to improve self-control to take into account its implications for people's broader lay theories of control. That is, to develop
  164. effective interventions for self-control, it may ultimately be important
  165. to understand how people's repeated experiences of exerting self-control in the context of the interventions themselves shape the development of their lay theories. The present findings illustrate the potential
  166. connection between these types of experiences and lay theories, but
  167. leave many other questions still unanswered. For example, it is not clear
  168. how long any changes in people's lay theories brought about by their
  169. experiences endure. Study 3 evaluated people's experiences over a twoweek period, but only assessed the influence of these experiences several days later. Presumably more prolonged and consistent experiences
  170. of effort and fatigue produce more prolonged and stable changes to
  171. people's lay theories, but these broader developmental questions need
  172. to be assessed in future research.
  173. In addition, more research is needed to understand the extent to
  174. which people connect the various experiences of effort and fatigue they
  175. have in their daily lives to their exertion of self-control. That is, in all
  176. three studies, participants were directly prompted to interpret their
  177. experiences in terms of self-control; we explicitly defined what we
  178. meant by self-control and, in Studies 2 and 3, emphasized that their
  179. assigned tasks would involve self-control. But, the question still remains
  180. under what conditions individuals spontaneously link experiences of
  181. effort and exhaustion on a given task to the exertion of self-control in
  182. their everyday life; and indeed, answering this question is an important
  183. step in developing effective interventions to improve self-control.
  184. In conclusion, the studies presented here provide new insights into
  185. how the experiences of effort and exhaustion influence people's
  186. thoughts about and enactment of self-control. Not only may experiencing high levels of fatigue serve as a signal for how to allocate one's
  187. effort and attention, as suggested by previous research (Brehm & Self,
  188. 1989; Clarkson et al., 2010; Kivetz & Zheng, 2006; Muraven et al.,
  189. 2008), but these experiences may also have broader consequences
  190. concerning the lay theories people form about self-control as something
  191. that is limited and must be conserved when the demands for it get too
  192. high. The present work thus helps to further our understanding of why
  193. self-control may be so difficult to sustain over time, and suggests important new directions for exploring methods for reducing this difficulty and bolstering self-control
Advertisement
Add Comment
Please, Sign In to add comment
Advertisement