Guest User

Olfactory Memory in Humans

a guest
Mar 30th, 2020
61
0
Never
Not a member of Pastebin yet? Sign Up, it unlocks many cool features!
text 12.47 KB | None | 0 0
  1. Olfactory Memory in Humans
  2. from The Temporal Lobe and Limbic System by Pierre Gloor, pages 316-318
  3.  
  4. [Entered by hand, likely many typos]
  5.  
  6. In humans, odors are reputed to have a particularly potent ability to evoke
  7. old memories of events or situations that usually occured in a socioaffective
  8. context of which that odor had been a part. Memories evoked by such odors are
  9. said to have an immediacy and vividness that is rarely equalled by those
  10. evoked by other sensory stimuli. I personally had an experience of this kind:
  11. Upon entering a room at a friend's country house I was struck by an odor that
  12. I immediately recognized as that of my grandmother's kitchen, into which I had
  13. not set foot since my childhood. The odor suddenly transported me back in
  14. time and space to that kitchen with an overwhelming feeling of familiarity.
  15. It was a place that under ordinary circumstances I thought about only on rare
  16. occasions, and these recollections never evoked this feeling of being there.
  17. Also, the odor would not have been accessible to my voluntary recall.
  18. Anecdotal reports like this illustrate two striking features peculiar to the
  19. olfactory sense: the durability of olfactory memories, which contrasts with
  20. the common inability to reactivate them voluntarily, and the associative power
  21. of the olfactory sense.
  22.  
  23. The most famous and often cited example of such a vivid recall of an old
  24. memory by an olfacto-gustatory sensation is that described in the first volume
  25. of Marcel Proust's novel A la Recherche du Temps Perdu (Remembrance of Things
  26. Past). Even though this is a literary and not a scientifically documented
  27. example it is worth reviewing, because it bears such an uncanny resemblance to
  28. the experiential response elicited by the electrical stimulation of the
  29. amygdala that is described in Chapter 1 of this book. In Proust's novel the
  30. narrator relates an experience elicited by the olfacto-gustatory sensation of
  31. tasting a certain kind of cookie, "une petite madeleine" dunked in tea that
  32. was offered to him by his mother. As soon as he put it into his mouth he was
  33. seized by a strong but isolated delicious feeling of pleasure that permeated
  34. him without any notion of what its source was ("un plaisir délicieux m'avait
  35. envahi, isolé, sans la notion de sa cause")--a type of "tip of the tongue"
  36. phenomenon that, in spite of its vividness, was initially impervious to any
  37. attempt at identifying its origin. In some way the flavor seemed to be
  38. connected to a visual memory that was trying to surface in his consciousness,
  39. and then suddenly that memory emerged with an overwhelmingly vivid clarity: it
  40. was that of an event in the narrator's childhood, when while visiting his aunt
  41. Léonie in the little town of Combray, she had offered him a "petite madeleine"
  42. that she had soaked in her tea. And with this memory there resurfaced a most
  43. vivid visual recall of his aunt's house, which like a stage set became
  44. superimposed upon the present scene (which had in the meanwhile been altered),
  45. together with the memories of the little town of Combray, of some of the
  46. places there, and of what he used to do there as a child. The narrator makes
  47. the point that he had many times seen "petites madeleines" in pastry shops and
  48. that the mere sight of them, there or even now in the incident he describes,
  49. had never evoked any memory. It was only when he tasted the madeleine and
  50. experienced its flavor--its odor and taste--that the memory surfaced in his
  51. mind. The sequence described by Proust bears a striking resemblance to that
  52. elicited by electrical stimulation of the amygdala in the epileptic patient,
  53. which is described in some detail in Chapter 1. There, as in Proust's
  54. literary example, the evocation of a remote childhood memory was at first
  55. unfocused yet strong, with a "tip of the tongue" quality, and upon
  56. restimulation there emergged the memory of reliving the old childhood event
  57. with a startling vividness and immediacy.
  58.  
  59. Proust's description and those of similar experiences of odor-evoked memories
  60. reported by others are thus in many ways similar to those some temporal lobe
  61. epileptics give of the experiential hallucinations that usher in their
  62. seizures: What characterizes this Proustian phenomenon is a sudden
  63. overwhelming and compellingly vivid quality that calls forth memories coupled
  64. with a feeling of reliving a past experience that is utterly inaccessible to
  65. voluntary recall. Are thus odors capable of tapping into the same memory
  66. evocation mechanism that is activated in some tmporal lobe epileptics by
  67. temporal lobe seizure discharge or by electrical stimulation of temporal lobe
  68. structures?
  69.  
  70. A scientific approach to olfactory memory cannot, however, rely on such
  71. anecdotal evidence. An attempt has been made to document objectively the
  72. power of odors to evoke vivid memories of past events and situations (Herz and
  73. Cupchick, 1992). College-student volunteers of both sexes were presented with
  74. a series of odors and asked to describe any memories that were evoked by any
  75. of them. This occurred commonly and such memories frequently had emotional
  76. connotations; were particularly clear; were usually not about events
  77. frequently recalled by the subjects in their everyday life; and were
  78. comparatively old, most often going back to early childhood. This study thus
  79. lends some experimental support to the notion that odors have the power to
  80. evoke vivid memories from a remote past. However, it can be criticized on the
  81. grounds that only the effects of olfactory stimuli were studied, and thus no
  82. comparison is possible with the capacity of visual or auditory stimuli to
  83. evoke memories.
  84.  
  85. Other experimental studies have, however, shown that olfactory memories have
  86. some peculiarities that indeed set them apart from memories in other sense
  87. modalities. Testing of human memory--for instance, in the visual
  88. modality--often relies on verbal labeling of the memoranda. Yet, in the study
  89. of olfactory memories, naming is an inadequate tool. People in a laboratory
  90. testing situation have a poor ability to attach verbal labels to odors, even
  91. to those with which they are familiar. Although they readily assent to the
  92. name of a familiar odor when it is supplied to them by the experimenter, they
  93. are perplexed that they had been unable to think of it themselves (Engen,
  94. 1987). Odors thus do not seem to stand on their own as individual,
  95. well-circumscribed percepts, in contrast to visual objects and sounds. This,
  96. besides accounting for the naming difficulty, may explain why it is difficult
  97. to conjure up in one's mind a well-known odor, whereas we have no difficulty
  98. in imagining visual objects or sounds. These peculiarities of olfactory
  99. memory are probably explainable by the fact that in the olfactory system,
  100. unlike in the visual system, elementary perceptual attributes are not treated
  101. separately from global attributes. There is nothing fundamentally different
  102. in the way the perceptual attributes of a single chemical and those of a
  103. mixture of chemicals are represented in the olfactory cortex: right from the
  104. entry stage into the central nervous system olfactory stimuli are represented
  105. non-topographically by a widely distributed matrix. Thus, a perception caused
  106. by a mixture of odors cannot be broken down into its simpler constituent
  107. elements as is the case, for instance, in the visual system. Herein lies on
  108. of the strengths, but also one of the weaknesses, of the olfactory sense.
  109. Even though by representing odors in "gestalt" form as distributed matricies
  110. right from their port of entry into the brain, the olfactory system is capable
  111. of representing and discriminating myriad different smells by very economic
  112. means, it is weak in analytical power. It cannot break down complex odors
  113. into its constituents. Odors seem to acquire significance for people in
  114. relation to the context in which they experience them, but not in relation to
  115. an abstract semantic label such as a name. People are thus similar to animals
  116. in this respect: odors acquire their significance through contextual cues. As
  117. discussed earlier, in macrosmatic animals, odors--in constrast to vemeronasal
  118. stimuli--commonly have no innate behavioral significance; they acquire it
  119. readily, however, through association, particularly in behavioral contexts
  120. that are highly significant to the animal.
  121.  
  122. When olfactory memory is tested in humans with methods that utilize odor
  123. recognition rather than naming, olfactory memory differs in two respects from
  124. visual memory: its acquisition is relatively poor as compared with visual
  125. memory, but its staying power by contrast is remarkable. Thus, at very short
  126. intervals--where the recognition of visual stimuli presented to a normal
  127. subject is nearly 100%--memory is poorer for odors, where at best it reaches
  128. 90% and frequently is between 70% and 85% (Engen et al, 1973; Lawless and
  129. Cain, 1975). But visual recognition memory fades fast and drops to chance
  130. levels within 3 months (Shepard, 1967); olfactory recognition memory shows ony
  131. a slight decay and then remains on a plateau of 70% for months or even longer
  132. (Engen and Ross, 1973; Lawless and Cain, 1975; Engen et al, 1991).
  133. Familiarity, pleasantness or unpleasantness of the odor, or whether it can be
  134. labeled by the subject, as well as the retention interval, has no influence on
  135. the persistence of the memory. This extraordinary staying power of olfactory
  136. memory coupled with its strong contextual associative properties probably lies
  137. at the root of the Proustian phenomenon. The phylogenetic history of
  138. olfaction suggests that this sense modality has been singled out in early
  139. mammalian evolution to subserve the role of relating olfactory stimuli to
  140. experiences that are highly significant to the animal in such behavioral
  141. situations as, for instance, procreation and various forms of social
  142. intercourse. Olfactory stimuli quickly form strong associaitions with such
  143. highly significant individually experienced events as shown by examples cited
  144. earlier. Such associations are very enduring. This role of olfaction may be
  145. less obvious in humans, but it survives.
  146.  
  147. There are probably nonolfactory memories that are as unforgettable as
  148. olfactory ones. They do not relate to isolated visual or auditory percepts,
  149. but to stimuli contextually imbedded in the experience of highly meaningful
  150. events of an individual's past. These are often either highly emotional
  151. memories or memories of childhood going back to a time when the fundamental
  152. experiences that shape one's socioaffective connections to the social nexus in
  153. which we are imbedded are formed, and are often concerned with kinship and
  154. territoriality (the home or other places that have a "territorial flavor"
  155. efining one's social nexus) and to frightening antagonistic encounters. Such
  156. memories are not necessarily limited to childhood experiences. Memories of
  157. this kind tend to be very enduring and seem to be as indestructible as
  158. olfactory memories. They are part of what defines our identity to ourselves.
  159. These are the type of memories that are reactivated in some temporal lobe
  160. epileptics by epileptic discharge or electrical stimulation of the temporal
  161. lobe structures, mostly the limbic structures of the temporal lobe. Olfactory
  162. stimuli and complex constellations of nonolfactory stimuli occurring in
  163. meaningful socioaffective contexts probably form enduring memories through
  164. interactions with both the amygdaloid and hippocampal systems. Reactivation
  165. of such memories by re-experiencing the underlying perceptual context of a
  166. fragment of it (particularly if that fragment is an olfactory stimulus) has
  167. the power to reactivate such memories, sometimes as in Proust's example (and
  168. replicated by the results of amygdaloid stimulation in the patient described
  169. in Chapter 1) by arousing its affective component first. Observations on the
  170. evocations of such memories by electrical stimulation suggest that the
  171. structure that is most prone to trigger such recalls in the amygdala (Feindel
  172. and Penfield, 1954; Gloor et al, 1981, 1982; Fish et al, 1993). From a
  173. comparative biological perspective this appears understandable because the
  174. amygdala is the locus where in macrosmatic animals the simplest associations
  175. of this kind--those between behaviorally meaningful pheromonal stimuli
  176. mediated by the vemeronasal system and olfactory stimuli encoding individual
  177. life experience mediated by the main olfactory system--take place.
Add Comment
Please, Sign In to add comment