Advertisement
Guest User

There is nothing paranormal about near-death experiences

a guest
Oct 11th, 2012
345
0
Never
Not a member of Pastebin yet? Sign Up, it unlocks many cool features!
text 14.88 KB | None | 0 0
  1. There is nothing paranormal about near-death
  2. experiences: how neuroscience can explain seeing
  3. bright lights, meeting the dead, or being convinced
  4. you are one of them
  5.  
  6. Dean Mobbs1 and Caroline Watt2
  7. 1 Medical Research Council, Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, 15 Chaucer Road, Cambridge, CB2 7EF, UK
  8. 2 University of Edinburgh, Department of Psychology, 7 George Square, Edinburgh, EH8 9JZ, UK
  9.  
  10. Approximately 3% of Americans declare to have had a
  11. near-death experience [1]. These experiences classically
  12. involve the feeling that one’s soul has left the body,
  13. approaches a bright light and goes to another reality,
  14. where love and bliss are all encompassing. Contrary to
  15. popular belief, research suggests that there is nothing
  16. paranormal about these experiences. Instead, neardeath
  17. experiences are the manifestation of normal brain
  18. function gone awry, during a traumatic, and sometimes
  19. harmless, event.
  20. The notion that death represents a new beginning, a
  21. passing to an afterlife, where we are reunited with loved
  22. ones and live eternally in a utopian paradise, is common
  23. across most theological doctrines. The theological case is
  24. solidified by the anecdotal reports of people who have
  25. encountered near-death experiences. These experiences
  26. leave an indelible mark on the individual’s life, often
  27. reducing any pre-existing fear of death. Such experiences
  28. have been observed across cultures and can be found in
  29. literature dating back to ancient Greece, but are placed
  30. firmly in the contemporary conscience by books, such as
  31. Raymond Moody’s Life after Life, which document many
  32. cases of near-death experiences. This and other bestseller
  33. books have largely omitted discussion of any physiological
  34. basis for these experiences, and instead appear to prefer
  35. paranormal explanations over and above scientific enlightenment.
  36. Yet, a handful of scientific studies of near-death experiences
  37. do exist. One example is a case study in which a
  38. patient with diabetes reported a near-death experience
  39. during an episode of hypoglycaemia (too low blood sugar).
  40. During this episode, the patient was in a sleep-like state
  41. with rapid eye-movement (REM) – a common marker of
  42. dreaming and thought to underlie the consolidation of
  43. memories, a process that may explain life reviews during
  44. near-death experiences (e.g. where the individual reexperiences
  45. or relives events from their life). Despite not
  46. being in danger of dying, upon resuscitation, the patient
  47. recounted many of the classic features of the near-death
  48. experience [2]. Indeed, Owen and colleagues reported that
  49. 30 out of 58 patients (51.7%) who recounted near-death
  50. experiences were not in medical danger, and hence death
  51. was not so near [3] (Figure 1).
  52.  
  53. Basic features of near-death experiences
  54. There are a number of common features across near-death
  55. experiences. In one study [2], of those who had had a neardeath
  56. experience, 50% reported an awareness of being
  57. dead, 24% said that they had had an out-of-body experience,
  58. 31% remembered moving through a tunnel, and 32%
  59. reported meeting with deceased people. Moreover, while it
  60. is a common anecdote that near-death experiences are
  61. associated with feelings of euphoria and bliss, only 56%
  62. associated the experience with such positive emotions, and
  63. some even reported negative experiences.
  64.  
  65. An awareness of being dead
  66. One of the most frequently reported features of near-death
  67. experiences is an awareness of being dead [2]. These feelings,
  68. however, are not limited to near-death experiences.
  69. For example, an intriguing syndrome, which can also help
  70. to explain the sensations relating to near-death experiences,
  71. is ‘Cotard’ or ‘walking corpse’ syndrome. This syndrome
  72. was named after the French neurologist Jules
  73. Cotard and results in ‘le de´lire de negation’ – a feeling
  74. that one is dead. McKay and Cipolotti published recently a
  75. case report on a 24-year old patient called LU with Cotard
  76. delusions [4]. LU repeatedly thought that she was in
  77. heaven (although actually in National Hospital, Queen
  78. Square, London) and that she might have died of flu.
  79. The delusions diminished over a few days and were gone
  80. after a week. Anatomically, Cotard syndrome has been
  81. associated with the parietal cortex, as well as the prefrontal
  82. cortex [5], and has been described following trauma,
  83. during advanced stages of typhoid and multiple sclerosis.
  84. Still, why delusions such as Cotard syndrome occur is
  85. unknown. One explanation is that they may simply be
  86. an attempt to make sense of the strange experiences that
  87. the patient is having.
  88.  
  89. Out-of-body experiences
  90. Out-of-body experiences are often described as feelings
  91. that one is floating outside of the body and in some cases
  92. involve ‘autoscopy’ or seeing one’s body from above. The
  93. celebrated Canadian neurosurgeon Wilder Penfield argued
  94. that the veridical perception during out-of-body experiences
  95. is brain-based. These experiences are also common during
  96. interrupted sleep patterns that occur just prior to sleep or
  97. waking. For example, sleep paralysis, which is a natural
  98. part of REM sleep and results in paralysis when the person
  99. is still aware of the external world, is reported in up to 40% of
  100. the population and is associated with hypnagogia (i.e. vivid
  101. dreamlike auditory, visual, or tactile hallucinations associated
  102. with the wake-sleep cycle), which can result in the
  103. sensation of floating above one’s body [6]. Recently, Olaf
  104. Blanke and colleagues demonstrated that out-of-body
  105. experiences can be artificially induced by stimulating the
  106. right temporoparietal junction [7]. Upon stimulation, the
  107. patient would say things like ‘‘I see myself lying in bed. . .’’.
  108. Other stimulations resulted in the patient feeling as if
  109. they were ‘floating’. The authors of the study suggested that
  110. out-of-body experiences result from a failure to integrate
  111. multisensory information from one’s body, which results
  112. in the disruption of the phenomenological elements of
  113. self-representation.
  114.  
  115. A tunnel of light
  116. Near-death experiences are also associated with the perception
  117. that one is moving down a dark tunnel and surfacing
  118. into a ‘world of light’. These experiences can also be
  119. artificially induced: pilots flying at G-force can sometimes
  120. experience a phenomenon known as hypotensive syncope,
  121. which usually causes tunnel-like peripheral to central
  122. visual loss to develop over 5-8 seconds [8]. Although in
  123. the case of actual near-death experiences the mechanisms
  124. are still unclear, a review by Nelson and colleagues suggested
  125. that the light at the end of the tunnel can be
  126. explained by visual activity during retinal ischemia [9],
  127. which occurs when the blood and oxygen supply to the eye
  128. is depleted. A visual disorder, such as glaucoma, can also
  129. result in loss of peripheral vision leading to tunnel vision.
  130. Indeed, such tunnel vision is associated with extreme fear
  131. and hypoxia (i.e. oxygen loss), two processes common to
  132. dying. As Blackmore points out, the visual cortex is organized
  133. by cells that process peripheral and fovea (i.e. central)
  134. vision and excitation of the these cells will result in a
  135. central bright light and dark periphery, that is, a tunnel
  136. effect [10].
  137.  
  138. Meeting deceased people
  139. Prevalent in fiction and celluloid is the notion that, when
  140. we die, we are surrounded by the souls of the dead, angels
  141. or a religious figure in a peaceful transcendental place.
  142. Many neuroscientific studies have shown that brain pathology
  143. can lead to similar visions. For example, patients
  144. with Alzheimer’s or progressive Parkinson’s disease can
  145. have vivid hallucinations of ghosts or even monsters.
  146. Patients have also been noted as seeing headless corpses
  147. and dead relatives in the house, which has been linked to
  148. pallidotomy lesions, suggesting that this result from abnormal
  149. dopamine functioning, a neurochemical that can
  150. evoke hallucinations[11]. Intriguingly, electrical stimulation
  151. of the adjacent region of the angular gyrus can result
  152. in a sense of presence (i.e. someone is standing behind
  153. us[12]). Macular degeneration (i.e. damage to the center, or
  154. macula, of the visual field) can also result in vivid visual
  155. hallucinations of ghosts and fairytale characters. Such an
  156. example is Charles-Bonnet syndrome, which mostly occurs
  157. in the elderly populations. One theory is that hallucinations
  158. occur due to compensatory over-activation in brain
  159. structures nearby the damaged area or making sense of
  160. noise coming from the damaged areas. Thus, hallucinations
  161. might be derived from internal sources which are
  162. appraised incorrectly.
  163.  
  164. Figure 1. Large scale studies showing the number of people who report near-death experiences (NDEs). The far left bars show that a large proportion (82%) of near-death
  165. survivors do not report near-death experiences [2]. The central bars represent a retrospective, or historic, study [15]. Crucially, the far right bars show that around half of the
  166. people who reported near-death experiences in this study were not in danger of dying [3].
  167. Update Trends in Cognitive Sciences October 2011, Vol. 15, No. 10
  168. 448
  169.  
  170. FIGURE FOUND AT: http://imgur.com/YHVFt
  171.  
  172. Positive emotions
  173. Another commonly reported feature of near-death experiences
  174. is a feeling of pure bliss, euphoria and an acceptance
  175. of death. Many medicinal and recreational drugs, however,
  176. can mirror the positive emotions and visions reported in
  177. near-death experiences. At varying doses, the administration
  178. of ketamine can mimic these experiences including
  179. hallucinations, out-of-body experiences, positive emotions
  180. such as euphoria, dissociation, and spiritual experiences.
  181. Ketamine is sometimes used as an anesthetic through its
  182. binding with opioid mu- receptors and hallucinations may
  183. occur through inhibiting N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA)
  184. receptors, the same receptors that are evoked during the
  185. administration of recreational drugs like amphetamine.
  186. Thus, the neurochemical processes that ketamine evokes
  187. may be similar to some of the positive emotional experiences
  188. and visions during near-death experiences, through
  189. evoking the rewarding properties of the opioid system and
  190. misattribution of events due to disruption of the prefrontal
  191. cortex [13]. Such processes may occur naturally and similar
  192. systems are evoked when animals are under extreme
  193. danger. For example, dopamine and opioid systems become
  194. active when an animal is under predatory attack. Thus,
  195. these endogenous systems come into play during highly
  196. traumatic events and have likely evolved to aid in the
  197. survival of the organism.
  198. Understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of
  199. near-death experiences
  200. The near-death experience is a complex set of phenomena
  201. and a single account will not capture all its components.
  202. One recent theory is that the basic arousal systems beginning
  203. in the midbrain may account for many of the components
  204. of the near-death experience [10]. Of interest is the
  205. locus coeruleus, a midbrain region involved in the release
  206. of noradrenaline. Noradrenaline is known to be involved in
  207. arousal related to fear, stress, and hypercarbia [10], and is
  208. highly connected to regions that mediate emotion and
  209. memory, including the amygdala and hippocampus. Indeed,
  210. stimulation of the noradrenaline system has been
  211. shown to enhance and consolidate memory, and plays a
  212. critical role in the sleep-wake cycle, including REM sleep.
  213. Along with basic midbrain systems, such as the periaqueductal
  214. gray, a region involved in opioid analgesia and basic
  215. fear responses, and the ventral tegmental area, which is a
  216. core dopamine reward area, the noradrenaline system may
  217. be part of a basic set of systems that directly or indirectly
  218. evoke positive emotions, hallucinations and other features
  219. of the near-death experience.
  220. Taken together, the scientific evidence suggests that all
  221. aspects of the near-death experience have a neurophysiological
  222. or psychological basis: the vivid pleasure frequently
  223. experienced in near-death experiences may be the result of
  224. fear-elicited opioid release, while the life review and REM
  225. components of the near-death experience could be attributed
  226. to the action of the locus coeruleus- noradrenaline
  227. system. Out-of-body experiences and feelings of disconnection
  228. with the physical body could arise because of a breakdown
  229. in multisensory processes, and the bright lights and
  230. tunneling could be the result of a peripheral to fovea
  231. breakdown of the visual system through oxygen deprivation.
  232. A priori expectations, where the individual makes
  233. sense of the situation by believing they will experience the
  234. archetypal near-death experience package, may also play a
  235. crucial role [14]. If one challenge of science is to demystify
  236. the world, then research should begin to test these and
  237. other hypotheses. Only then will discussion of near-death
  238. experiences move beyond theological dialogue and into the
  239. lawful realm of empirical neurobiology.
  240.  
  241. Acknowledgements
  242. We wish to thank Chris Frith, Bernhard Staresina, Donna Harris and
  243. Michael Ewbank for helpful comments. The paper was supported by the
  244. UK Medical Research Council.
  245.  
  246. References
  247. 1 Schmied, I. et al. (1999) Todesna¨heerfahrungen in Ost- und
  248. Westdeutschland – eine empirische Untersuchung. In Todesna¨he:
  249. interdisziplina¨re Zuga¨nge zu einem außergewo¨hnlichen Pha¨nomen
  250. (Knoblaub, H. and Soeffner, H.G., eds), pp. 217–250, Universita¨tsverlag
  251. Konstanz
  252. 2 van Lommel, P. et al. (2001) Near-Death experiences in surviviors of
  253. cardiac arrest: a prospective study in the Netherlands. Lancet 358,
  254. 2039–2045
  255. 3 Owens, J.E. et al. (1990) Features of near-death experiences in relation
  256. to whether or not the patients were near death. Lancet 336, 1175–1177
  257. 4 McKay, R. and Cipolotti, L. (2007) Attributional style in a case of
  258. Cotard delusion. Conscious. Cogn. 16, 349–359
  259. 5 Feinberg, T.E. and Keenan, J.P. (2005) Where in the brain is the self?
  260. Conscious. Cogn. 14, 661–678
  261. 6 Cheyne, J.A. and Girard, T.A. (2009) The body unbound: Vestibularmotor
  262. hallucinations and out-of-body experiences. Cortex 45, 201–215
  263. 7 Blanke, O. and Arzy, S. (2004) The out-of-body experience: disturbed
  264. self-processing at the temporo-parietal junction. Neuroscientist 11,
  265. 16–24
  266. 8 Lambert, E.H. and Wood, E.H. (1946) Direct determination of man’s
  267. blood pressure on the human centrifuge during positive acceleration.
  268. Federation Proc. 5, 59
  269. 9 Nelson, K.R. et al. (2007) Out-of-body experience and arousal.
  270. Neurology 68, 794–795
  271. 10 Blackmore, S.J. and Troscianko, T. (1988) The Physiology of the
  272. Tunnel. J. Near Death Stud. 8, 15–28
  273. 11 Manford, M. and Andermann, F. (1998) Complex visual hallucinations:
  274. Clinical and neurobiological insights. Brain 121, 1819–1840
  275. 12 Blanke, O. et al. (2002) Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions.
  276. Nature 419, 269–270
  277. 13 Liao, Y. et al. (2011) Reduced dorsal prefrontal gray matter after
  278. chronic ketamine use. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 42–48
  279. 14 Blackmore, S.J. (1993) Dying to live: Science and the near-death
  280. experience, Grafton
  281. 15 Ring, K. (1980) Life at death: a scientific investigation of the near-death
  282. experience, Coward McCann and Geoghenan
Advertisement
Add Comment
Please, Sign In to add comment
Advertisement