Advertisement
Guest User

Birds and patterns

a guest
Apr 7th, 2013
408
0
Never
Not a member of Pastebin yet? Sign Up, it unlocks many cool features!
text 12.55 KB | None | 0 0
  1. Excerpted from James Paul Gee's "What Video Games Have to Teach Us About Learning and Literacy":
  2.  
  3.  
  4. If the human mind is a powerful pattern recognizer—and the evidence very much suggests it is—
  5. then what it most important about thinking is not that it is “mental,” something
  6. happening inside our heads, but rather that it is social, something
  7. attuned to and normed by the social groups to which we belong or seek to
  8. belong. Since this statement so violates our commonsense notions of psychology,
  9. let me hasten to explain.
  10.  
  11. Let’s, for a moment, consider a birdwatching club. Birdwatchers are
  12. good at seeing some elements (features) of birds and quickly extending the
  13. pattern to name a particular type of bird. Thus they may see a flash of graybrown
  14. and a splash of white under the body as a small bird flits in and out of
  15. tall grass in an open grassland. They will, at least in much of the United
  16. States, conclude they have seen a bobolink, even if they have not seen all the
  17. bird’s other salient features. If they see a flash of brown-brown and a splash of
  18. white under the body of a slightly larger bird flitting among the trees of a
  19. forest, they will conclude they have seen a flicker, a type of woodpecker, even
  20. if they have not seen the bird’s distinctive purple spots and yellow shaft.
  21.  
  22. The first thing a birdwatching club needs to do is ensure that all its
  23. members have had the right experiences in the world to have formed such
  24. patterns and engaged in such ways of filling them out in the field. People can
  25. wander around on their own and experience—and pay attention to—all sorts
  26. of things to do with birds, many of them interesting, but not all that helpful if
  27. you want to be a contributing member of the birdwatching club. Perhaps
  28. every time you have hiked you have never seen birds in open grassland (easy
  29. to happen, unless you are out early in the morning or at dusk, when the birds
  30. are active and often sitting atop a grass stalk or small plant, since otherwise
  31. the birds are on the ground hidden below the tall grass). You conclude that
  32. there are no bird species that live in open grassland. You have formed a
  33. (wrong) pattern in which open grassland is negatively associated with birds.
  34. The bird club will see to it that you get out to some open grassland at the
  35. right time of day so you can see the bobolinks and meadowlarks. Why? Because
  36. they want you to have similar enough experiences as the other club
  37. members so that you can share knowledge with them.
  38.  
  39. The first thing the bird club does is see to it that new members come to
  40. share a lot of experiences of the world of birds and birding with the other
  41. members, so that they will share some common patterns and ways of completing
  42. them when they have seen only some features in a pattern. Then the
  43. members can make lists of all the birds around in the winter in a given part of
  44. the country or find out that birds that live in open grasslands are much more
  45. endangered than the forest songbirds that have received almost all the publicity
  46. concerning their supposed decline. Now they also can engage in competition
  47. as to who are the best birdwatchers in the sense of being able to
  48. identify the most different species of birds in a given place during a certain
  49. set amount of time, since they are now all adept at the basic practice and
  50. norms of the “game.”
  51.  
  52. But there is a second thing the bird club must do. Say Mary Smith, a
  53. member in good standing, all of a sudden comes to the club one day and says,
  54. given what she has seen out in the field, that she is pretty sure that what she
  55. saw was a dodo, an extinct species of bird. Perhaps, more realistically, Mary
  56. Smith keeps claiming to have seen ivory-billed woodpeckers (which are
  57. probably extinct, but we’re not sure) rather than pileated woodpeckers
  58. (which certainly exist, though they are not horribly common), based on a
  59. flicker of some distinctive features. (She never seems to get a full view of the
  60. bird before it flies off.) Both birds look similar, though ivory-billed woodpeckers
  61. are bigger and have a somewhat different bill. Given how unlikely it
  62. is that anyone will see an ivory-billed woodpecker (though, if they are not extinct,
  63. it is possible), the club will insist that any member who claims to have
  64. seen one has seen it well and very carefully checked the identification. (If you
  65. see a big black woodpecker with a red crest that looks a lot like Woody
  66. Woodpecker, it’s almost certainly a pileated woodpecker.)
  67.  
  68. If, in either case, dodo or ivory-billed woodpecker, Mary Smith persists,
  69. what happens? At first, the club refuses to publish her lists in its newsletter.
  70. Eventually, if she persists further, the club kicks her out. What’s going on
  71. here? The club is norming (yes, policing, if you like) its members’ patterns
  72. and ways of filling them out. If a member deviates too far from the patterns
  73. and ways of filling them out in the field that the club, as a social group, considers
  74. normative, then the club “punishes” the member in order to bring him
  75. or her back in line. It’s not that, if ivory-billed woodpeckers still exist, no
  76. member could see one and get it published in the club’s newsletter. But seeing
  77. an ivory-billed woodpecker, like seeing any bird in the context of birdwatching
  78. as a social practice, is not just a mental event, it’s also a social event.
  79. There are social rules or norms about what counts as having seen an ivorybilled
  80. woodpecker, and, in this case, they are strict.
  81.  
  82. The point of this diversion about birdwatching is this: The patterns and
  83. ways of filling them out that count are not really the ones inside the heads of
  84. the members (though they are there, of course). All the members, as individual
  85. human beings, have a myriad of patterns about birds, and ways of filling
  86. them out in the field, in their heads. But the club, as a social group, has a set
  87. of norms and values that determines certain sorts of patterns and ways of filling
  88. them out in the field as “ideal” (central). This ideal might actually not be
  89. what is in anyone’s head. The ideal is an “attractor,” an ideal toward which
  90. individuals in the club gravitate and toward which the social practices (the
  91. “policing”) of the club pushes them when they get too far away from it.
  92.  
  93. If someone questions what constitutes the “right” pattern in regard to
  94. seeing an ivory billed woodpecker or, more realistically, what are the right patterns
  95. in terms of which one can identify the many types of (similar-looking)
  96. sparrows or seagulls, people in the club don’t open up anyone’s head. They
  97. engage in dialogue (negotiation and contestation) with each other, inspect
  98. their practices, read their texts, and, yes, ask certain people what they think,
  99. probably the old-timers or “insiders” and not the newcomers or marginal
  100. members. In the end, if thinking is a matter of pattern reorganization and filling
  101. our patterns, then thinking is at least as much social as it is mental and individual.
  102. Actually, it is more social than mental and individual. Of course, this
  103. idea violates traditional ways of thinking about psychology. So be it.
  104.  
  105. Even if you are not into birds, you surely have seen many sparrows and
  106. you have formed patterns in your mind in regard to them. You probably associate
  107. being little and brown with being a type of sparrow very strongly. You
  108. may well associate being in a city or in the yard of a suburban house with
  109. being a sparrow. You may, in terms of the patterns in your mind, recognize a
  110. few different types of sparrows (perhaps you have noticed that some in your
  111. yard have a little yellow cap on their head and some don’t), or maybe you
  112. don’t and see them all as basically the same type of bird. You have a set of elements
  113. associated with sparrows in your head, and you associate them more
  114. or less strongly with each other and with being a sparrow.
  115.  
  116. These sparrow patterns you have in your head, which are perfectly normal
  117. for “everyday” people, will not work for being a “birder.” From the
  118. standpoint of a birdwatching club, they are the wrong patterns, and the club,
  119. should you seek to join it, would help to give you other experiences that
  120. would shift the patterns in your head more toward its ideal. For people who
  121. have been normed by other birdwatchers and their practices and texts, their
  122. heads contain patterns in terms of which there are a great many different
  123. types of sparrows, many of which look quite a bit a like. They associate a
  124. great many of these with nonurban and nonsuburban settings, and, in fact,
  125. they associate the common house sparrow with finches and not New World
  126. sparrows at all.
  127.  
  128. Since the world is replete with features and people are powerful pattern
  129. recognizers, anyone can form almost any sort of interesting pattern in his or
  130. her head—forming all sorts of concepts and subconcepts. None of this is
  131. “right” or “wrong” until we ask which social group helps to norm (or police)
  132. patterns (concepts) in this or that domain that we wish to be part of. If you
  133. want to join a birdwatching club, you have to admit that the patterns in your
  134. head about sparrows are wrong; if you don’t want to join the club, then the
  135. patterns may very well be just fine from the point of view of some other
  136. group, if only your own culture, community, or family. Again, the patterns
  137. are in our heads, but they become meaningful (“right” or “wrong”) only from
  138. the perspective of the workings of social groups that “enforce” certain patterns
  139. as ideal norms toward which everyone in that group should orient (even
  140. if the patterns in their heads never resemble the ideal perfectly). Of course,
  141. for some purposes, though perhaps not for others, some groups’ practices
  142. work better than do those of other groups.
  143.  
  144. What I have said about our birdwatching club is true of any group. We
  145. humans belong to a myriad of social and cultural groups. Some of these
  146. groups are families and communities of various sizes. Some are cultural
  147. groups defined in various ways. Some are what I have called in previous chapters
  148. “affinity groups.” (The birdwatching club would be an affinity group.)
  149.  
  150. Affinity groups are groups wherein people primarily orient toward a common
  151. set of endeavors and social practices in terms of which they attempt to
  152. realize these endeavors. In such groups people orient less towards shared
  153. gender, race, culture, or face-to-face relationships, although all of these can
  154. play a secondary role. People can be in affinity groups where they rarely see
  155. many of the members face-to-face (e.g., the group may communicate in part
  156. at a distance via media, whether it’s print, the Internet, or what have you).
  157. Adrian was part of several different overlapping affinity groups in his work
  158. and play with video games.
  159.  
  160. Could a person think and reason outside the scope of any group, that is,
  161. with no group serving to norm his or her patterns and ways of filling them
  162. out? As Wittgenstein pointed out long ago, in a different context, such people
  163. would have no way to accurately test whether their patterns were veridical.
  164. Of course, the world would speak back when they tried to operate in and
  165. on it in terms of their patterns. But this would just be more experience of the
  166. world in terms of which their patterns were formed in the first place. Only
  167. when others have normed our patterns and ways of filling them out, so that
  168. we can be fairly confident that we are not “fooling” ourselves because of our
  169. own self-interest, desires, or idiosyncratic ways, can we trust our patterns in a
  170. particular domain.
  171.  
  172. Does this mean that no one can think an “original” thought? Of course
  173. not. But all the scientists, for example, who have thought original thoughts
  174. that their fellow scientists have never (eventually) come to see as close to the
  175. ideal patterns in their domain have thought thoughts that, at least of yet, no
  176. one knows about. As of yet, at least, their thoughts don’t “count” as part of
  177. their domain and are not “published” (spread in speech or writing).
  178.  
  179. And, of course, what counts to a group as an ideal pattern and way of filling
  180. it out in new experiences changes with time. I have already pointed out
  181. that the norms and values of groups are contested and negotiated. They are
  182. no more stable than is our concept of a bedroom (which we can shift when I
  183. link a hot plate to the room), and for the same reason. When the group confronts
  184. a new experience (in the world or in ideas), this experience can change
  185. the links (associations) among all the elements in the patterns the group considers
  186. ideal or normative, though this change happens through dialogue in
  187. speech and writing, not just via private thoughts.
Advertisement
Add Comment
Please, Sign In to add comment
Advertisement